Introduction
In Canada, in 2019, 32% of police-reported hate crimes were motivated by religion (Moreau, 2021; Current Study Table 1). Despite a marked decrease in hate crimes against Muslims from 2017 to 2018, they were still the second most targeted religious group. Muslims in Canada have historically been treated poorly. Specifically, one-third of Muslims in Canada in the past 5 years remark that they have experienced discrimination or have been treated unreasonably because of their religion, language, or cultural background (Environics Institute, 2016). Muslims were overrepresented as victims of hate crimes as early as 2011. Specifically, Canadian Muslims represented 20% of all victims of hate crime motivated by religion, while representing only 3.2% of the population (Allen, 2015). Further, hate-crimes targeting Muslims in Canada increased by 9% from the previous year (Moreau, 2021). In a study among Ontario and Québec Muslims, Ameli and Merali (2014) found that 11% of respondents had experienced some form of physical assault. The prevalence of violent hate crimes against Muslims is nearly ten-fold when compared to level 1 assaults—which includes physical assault and verbal threats—in the general Canadian population which is less than 1% (Statistics Canada, 2020). Further, Muslims are more likely to experience hate crimes that are violent in nature. These hate crimes disproportionately affect Muslim women, highlighting a heightened victimization risk due to the intersection of ethnicity and gender (Perry, 2014; Moreau, 2021). Canadian Muslims are additionally more likely to experience hate crimes due to the intersection of their religion and ethnicity. In Canada, 90% of Muslims are members of a visible minority1 group. Specifically, a third of Canadian Muslims are of South Asian background, a third of Arab background, and a third of other visible minority groups (Allen, 2015). This suggests that religion may work in conjunction with race and ethnicity in motivating hate crimes against Canadian Muslims. Indeed, in 2019, of the 46% of hate crimes based on race or ethnicity, Arab and West Asian Canadians were amongst the visible minority groups most frequently targeted (Moreau, 2021).
2014 |
2015 |
2016 |
2017 |
2018 |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motivation |
# |
% |
# |
% |
# |
% |
# |
% |
# |
% |
Race or ethnic origin (total) |
611 |
48 |
641 |
48 |
666 |
48 |
878 |
43 |
780 |
44 |
Black |
238 |
19 |
224 |
17 |
214 |
15 |
321 |
16 |
283 |
16 |
East or South East Asian |
52 |
4 |
49 |
4 |
61 |
4 |
61 |
3 |
56 |
3 |
South Asian |
49 |
4 |
48 |
4 |
72 |
5 |
67 |
3 |
75 |
4 |
Arab or West Asian |
69 |
5 |
92 |
7 |
112 |
8 |
142 |
7 |
111 |
6 |
Native American |
37 |
3 |
35 |
3 |
30 |
2 |
31 |
2 |
35 |
2 |
White |
49 |
4 |
38 |
3 |
36 |
3 |
46 |
2 |
37 |
2 |
Other race or ethnic origin1 |
106 |
8 |
130 |
10 |
125 |
9 |
169 |
8 |
159 |
9 |
Race or ethnic origin not specified |
11 |
1 |
25 |
2 |
16 |
1 |
41 |
2 |
24 |
1 |
Religion (total) |
429 |
34 |
469 |
35 |
460 |
33 |
842 |
41 |
639 |
36 |
Jewish |
213 |
17 |
178 |
13 |
221 |
16 |
360 |
18 |
347 |
20 |
Muslim |
99 |
8 |
159 |
12 |
139 |
10 |
349 |
17 |
173 |
10 |
Catholic |
35 |
3 |
55 |
4 |
27 |
2 |
39 |
2 |
35 |
2 |
Other religion |
49 |
4 |
41 |
3 |
37 |
3 |
57 |
3 |
46 |
3 |
Religion not specified |
33 |
3 |
36 |
3 |
36 |
3 |
37 |
2 |
38 |
2 |
Sexual orientation |
155 |
12 |
141 |
11 |
176 |
13 |
204 |
10 |
173 |
10 |
Other motivation |
77 |
6 |
86 |
6 |
88 |
6 |
117 |
6 |
171 |
10 |
Unknown motivation |
23 |
N/A |
25 |
N/A |
19 |
N/A |
32 |
N/A |
35 |
N/A |
Total |
1295 |
100 |
1362 |
100 |
1409 |
100 |
2073 |
100 |
1798 |
100 |
1Includes motivations based on race or ethnicity not reported elsewhere (e.g., Latin American, South American), as well as hate crimes that target more than one race or more than one ethnic group.
2Includes motivations based on a religion not declared elsewhere (e.g., Sikh, Hindu, Buddhism).
3Inlcudes motivations based on mental or physical health disabilities, gender, age, and other similar factors (e.g., profession or political views).
Note: The information in this table corresponds to data reported by police services serving 99.95% of the Canadian population. Hate crimes for which the motive was unknown are excluded from the calculation of the percentages. Percentages may not add up to 100 due to rounding. Adapted from Statistique Canada, Centre canadien de la statistique juridique et de la sécurité des collectivités, Programme de déclaration uniforme de la criminalité fondé sur l’affaire. Retrieved from https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/fr/pub/85-002-x/2020001/article/00003-fra.pdf?st=9fKLkafM.
N/A = Unable to calculate.
Increase in Islamophobia in Canada
In the past few years alone, there have been several high-profile cases of hate crimes against Canadian Muslims. On January 29, 2017, six Muslim people were shot dead in a Québec City Mosque (Austen & Smith, 2017). On September 12, 2020, a Muslim person standing outside of a mosque near Toronto was stabbed to death (CBC News, 2020). Additionally, on June 6, 2021, four Muslim people were killed by a driver who purposefully hit them with a car in London, Ontario (Westoll & LeBel, 2021). Though these are just a few high-profile incidents highlighted by Canadian media, these events are not isolated. Hate crime trends like those just mentioned have encouraged efforts by the Canadian government to “collect data to contextualize hate crime reports and to conduct needs assessments for impacted communities” (Canada Parliament, House of Commons, Member motion, 2017). These efforts include assessing different sources of information pertaining to hate crimes, including police reports and surveys.
While police-recorded hate crimes represent one source of information on the experiences of the Canadian Muslim community, additional evidence on the occurrence, nature, and impact of hate crimes is provided by self-reporting and media reports. For example, a survey of Canadian Muslims in 2002 by the Canadian Council on American Islamic Relations found that 56% of respondents experienced at least one anti-Muslim incident in the twelve months since 9/11 (CAIR-CAN, 2002). Further, the Canadian Islamic Congress (CIC, 2003) noted a 1600% increase in the incidence of anti-Muslim hate crimes reported to them following the incidents of 9/11. Comparatively, hate crimes against Muslims in the US have been found to be 5 times more common than before the 9/11 attacks (Samari, 2016). Additionally, in a poll recorded after 9/11, it was found that 60% of Americans reported adverse sentiments toward Muslims (Samari, 2016). Further, the Council on American-Islamic Relations reported that after the attacks in Paris, France on November 13, 2015, there was an increase in threats, violence, and discriminatory acts targeting Muslim Americans and those perceived as Muslim (Samari, 2016). In addition, negative media coverage contributes to the perpetuation of Islamophobic views because in social situations, Muslims are seen as the “enemy” (Considine, 2017). Further perpetuating Islamophobia, attacks committed by Muslims had an average of 449% more coverage in the media than attacks committed by non-Muslims (Considine, 2017). Negative media depictions and self-reported increases in Islamophobia post-9/11 may help explain the disproportionate hate crimes experienced by Muslims, not only in Canada but internationally as well.
Consequences of Islamophobia and Psychological Well-Being
Islamophobia against Muslim Canadians has been reported in the form of negative attitudes towards this marginalized population (Wilkins-Laflamme, 2018). The consequences of Islamophobia are widespread and impact many facets of well-being. Islamophobia leading to hate crimes can have negative impacts both socially and psychologically. For example, Muslim Canadians that have experienced hate crimes have reported economic burdens due to having to change jobs, having to use a car out of fear of harassment on the public transport system, and even having to change to more secure living locations (Mercier-Dalphond & Helly, 2021).
The economic burden from these hate crimes is not the only consequence of Islamophobia. Psychological distress and decreased well-being are also commonly reported effects of hate crimes (Lefevor et al., 2018; Padela & Heisler, 2011). Psychological distress is defined as non-specific symptoms of stress, anxiety, and depression such as feeling nervous, restless or fidgety, hopeless, sad, and worthless (Kessler et al., 2002; Viertiö et al., 2021). Further, psychological distress can also be defined as specific clinical features, including symptoms of depression, anxiety, and somatic symptoms (such as lack of energy or headaches) (Drapeau et al., 2012). Psychological distress can have far-reaching effects on overall health, including an increased mortality risk (Barry et al., 2020). Therefore, understanding the psychological consequences of Islamophobia and hate crimes is incredibly important.
Experiencing crime of any nature is distressing, but hate crimes are particularly damaging as victims are more likely to experience psychological distress, such as feeling nervous, anxious, or depressed; anger; and safety concerns compared to victims of other types of crime (Altemeyer, 1996; Boeckmann & Turpin-Petrosino, 2002; Gerstenfeld, 2011). For example, findings on the impacts of hate crimes against sexual and religious minorities in America indicate that victims of religiously-motivated hate crimes experience increased psychological distress (Lefevor et al., 2018; Padela & Heisler, 2011). This discrimination is not only associated with psychological distress but can lead to various other health problems like depression, anxiety, substance abuse, and suicide (Assari & Lankarani, 2017). Specifically, Lowe et al. (2018) found that discrimination against Muslim American college students was associated with a high mental health burden, with 42.9% of students being classified as having symptoms of major depression or generalized anxiety disorder.
Similarly, in Canada, there is evidence that the intersection of race and religion contributes to the experience of psychological distress. Specifically, Rousseau et al. (2011) found that psychological distress was significantly more elevated among Arab Canadians that were Muslim. However, these authors did not examine the impact of hate crimes on the psychological distress of these populations. Expanding on previous findings regarding experiences of distress among Canadian Muslims (Rousseau et al., 2011) and the mental health impacts of hate crimes on American Muslims (Lefevor et al., 2019, and Padela & Heisler, 2011), the current study assessed the psychological distress that follows hate crime experiences among Muslims in Canada. Psychological distress can have far-reaching effects on overall health (such as anxiety disorders) (Barry et al., 2020). Therefore, understanding the psychological impact of religion and race-based (such as Islamophobia motivated) hate crimes on Muslims in Canada is imperative to improving the health and well-being of racial and religious minority populations.
Present Study
The primary goals of the present study were to document and describe Canadian Muslims’ experiences of hate crime, including the extent, nature, and context of these crimes, and to examine the correlation of psychological distress after experiencing a hate crime. The first objective was to consider diversity in hate crime experiences among Muslim communities, for example, by considering differences based on gender, age, immigration history, ethnicity, and geographic region. The second objective was to consider the impacts of victimization on self-reported psychological distress. Specifically, the research questions were: 1) Which factors predict the probability of being a victim of verbal aggression (1a) or physical assault? (1b), and 2) Which factors predict overall psychological distress after victimization? This study addressed these questions based on survey data collected from Muslims across Canada. The results indicated that 74% of total participants reported experiencing verbal aggression, and 63% reported experiencing physical assaults. While there were no discernible risk factors for verbal aggression, living in Vancouver and self-identifying as visibly Muslim was associated with a greater likelihood of physical assault. Being physically assaulted, being a woman, and self-identifying as visibly Muslim were all associated with experiencing greater psychological distress after hate crime victimization. These findings provide a valuable snapshot of the experiences of Muslims living in Canada, further contributing to research on hate crimes against marginalized populations.
Measures and Methods
Sampling Strategy
To develop a random sample, we used an innovative sampling strategy known as Respondent-Driven Sampling (RDS). RDS is based on Markov chain mathematics and social networking theory, and has been successfully employed by researchers seeking to identify and conduct research with hard-to-reach populations that may be widely dispersed. Briefly, RDS begins by identifying a small number (15 in this case) of “seed” respondents in the target community. These seeds must be motivated individuals who will be asked to distribute the survey to three different people in the identified community, and so on. This method is described in greater detail in Perry and Alvi (2011).
For this survey, six Canadian cities were selected (Calgary, Edmonton, Montreal, Ottawa, Toronto, and Vancouver). The researchers, onsite research assistants, and members of local Muslim organizations facilitated the identification and solicitation of 15 initial seeds per city, emphasizing diverse representation from across Muslim communities. The seeds were preferentially selected based on whether they had a large social network, were respected members of the community, and would be able to recruit others to participate in the study (Heckathorn, 1997; 2002). The seeds were provided with a link for the online survey, as well as contact information for the research team, including community collaborators.
To be eligible to participate, the respondents had to identify as Muslim. To recruit survey respondents through chain referrals, all survey respondents, including the seeds, were asked to recruit three other individuals to complete the survey as well. Information about how to refer someone to the study was included in the survey. To prevent duplicate responses, each person was assigned a unique identification number that they entered in the survey. If participants did not have access to a computer or other device, participants were able to complete the survey at a partner agency site or at the university of one of the research team members.
Measures
The survey consisted of 48 questions about experiences related to being Muslim, being the victim of a hate crime, psychological distress, and sociodemographic information. The questions were adapted from a pilot study conducted in Ontario, Canada (Perry & Alvi, 2011). The survey was available in both French and English, and took approximately 45 minutes to complete.
Ethnic Identification
Participants were asked to identify their own ethnicities. These were then regrouped into 7 categories: Asian, Canadian/European, Maghreb, Middle Eastern, Sub-Saharan African, Turk, and Mixed ethnicity. Mixed ethnicity was given to any participant that indicated more than one ethnicity. These regrouped categories were recoded as dummy (binary) variables. For example, for Asian ethnicity ( yes = 1; no = 0). See Table 2 for demographics.
Sample demographics |
Canadian Muslim population1 |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Variable |
N |
% |
N |
% |
Gender |
||||
1. Men |
84 |
41.0 |
391,465 |
51.3 |
2. Women |
119 |
58.0 |
369,060 |
48.7 |
3. Other |
2 |
1.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
Total |
205 |
100.0 |
760,525 |
100.0 |
Born in Canada |
||||
1. Yes |
60 |
26.1 |
83,895 |
11.5 |
2. No |
170 |
73.9 |
643,330 |
88.5 |
Total |
230 |
100.0 |
727,225 |
100.0 |
Region |
||||
Calgary and Edmonton |
9 |
4.0 |
72,205 |
11.2 |
Ottawa |
22 |
9.8 |
46,835 |
7.3 |
Montréal |
62 |
27.7 |
155,340 |
24.1 |
Toronto |
50 |
22.3 |
311,700 |
48.4 |
Vancouver |
27 |
12.1 |
58,160 |
9.0 |
Total |
224 |
100.0 |
644,240 |
100.0 |
Age range |
||||
18–24 |
36 |
19.4 |
164,1552 |
22.9 |
25–30 |
26 |
14.0 |
176,1453 |
24.7 |
31–34 |
27 |
14.5 |
N/A |
N/A |
35–40 |
44 |
23.7 |
179,2604 |
25.1 |
41–44 |
19 |
10.2 |
N/A |
N/A |
45–50 |
20 |
10.8 |
126,0955 |
17.7 |
51+ |
14 |
6.1 |
68,4106 |
9.6 |
Total |
186 |
100.0 |
714,065 |
100.0 |
Education |
||||
No certificate, diploma, or degree |
2 |
1.0 |
59,370 |
10.8 |
High school diploma or equivalent |
14 |
6.9 |
101,220 |
18.4 |
Trades or apprenticeship certificate or diploma |
2 |
1.0 |
31,800 |
5.8 |
Certificate or diploma from a college, CEGEP, or other |
20 |
9.9 |
73,185 |
13.3 |
University certificate or diploma below bachelor’s degree |
10 |
4.9 |
43,820 |
8.0 |
Bachelor’s university degree |
71 |
35.0 |
126,195 |
22.9 |
Master’s university degree |
64 |
31.5 |
114,3157 |
20.8 |
Degree in medicine, dentistry, veterinary medicine |
2 |
1.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
Doctorate degree |
18 |
8.9 |
N/A |
N/A |
Total |
203 |
100.0 |
549,9108 |
100.0 |
Self-identified as visibly Muslim |
||||
1. Yes |
140 |
63.9 |
N/A |
N/A |
2. No |
79 |
36.1 |
N/A |
N/A |
Total |
219 |
100.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
Ethnic group |
||||
Asian (e.g. Afghan, Bangladeshi, Indian, Pakistani, etc.) |
63 |
27.3 |
N/A |
N/A |
European (e.g. Canadian, American, British, French, etc.) |
8 |
3.5 |
N/A |
N/A |
Maghreb (e.g. Algerian, Tunisian, Moroccan, etc.) |
44 |
19.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
Middle East (e.g. Arabic, Iranian, Iraqi, Lebanese, Syrian, etc.) |
37 |
16.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
Turk |
18 |
7.8 |
N/A |
N/A |
Sub-Saharan African (e.g. Ethiopian, Kenyan, Somalian, etc.) |
13 |
5.6 |
N/A |
N/A |
Mixed Ethnicity |
43 |
18.6 |
N/A |
N/A |
Total |
226 |
100.0 |
N/A |
N/A |
1Demographics derived from the Canadian National Household Survey, 2011 and are for people aged 15 years and over. The sample percentage is the valid percent and is based on the total listed.
2Ages 15–24.
3Ages 25–34.
4Ages 35–44.
5Ages 45–54.
6Ages 55–64.
7For education, the levels were based on those aged 25–64.
8This includes any university certificate higher than a bachelor’s degree.
9Demographics for the Canadian population ethnic groups are not grouped in a comparable way to the current manuscript groupings and therefore were not included. N/A = Not available.
Note:
Gender
Participants were asked to identify their gender (man = 1, woman = 0, other = NA). There were two cases that identified their gender as other and were therefore considered as “missing” in the analyses as there were too few to consider this third group.
Age Group
Participants were asked to identify their age group in years (18–24 = 1, 25–30 = 2, 31–34 = 3, 35–40 = 4, 41–44 = 5, 45–50 = 6, 51+= 7).
Born in Canada
Participants were asked whether they were born in Canada (yes = 1 or no = 0).
Region
Participants were asked to identify the region they were living in at the time of the survey (Calgary or Edmonton = 1, Ottawa = 2, Toronto = 3, Vancouver = 4, Montréal = 5). For the regression analyses, each of these levels were recoded into dummy variables (for example, lived in Ottawa (yes = 1; no = 0)).
Education
Participants were asked to indicate their highest educational attainment (No certificate, diploma, or degree = 1; High school diploma or equivalent = 2; Trades or apprenticeship certificate or diploma = 3; Certificate or diploma from a college, CEGEP, or other = 4; University certificate or diploma below bachelor’s degree = 5; Bachelor’s university degree = 6; Master’s university degree = 7; Degree in medicine, dentistry, veterinary medicine = 8; Doctorate degree = 9).
Visibly Muslim
Participants were asked if they felt they were easily identifiable as Muslim (yes = 1; no = 0).
Victimization
Participants were asked a total of 18 questions on how many times in the last year and since their arrival to Canada had they been verbally (for example, “received phone calls, texts, or emails that were humiliating or insulting”) or physically assaulted (for example, “had things purposely thrown at you”). If participants stated that an event occurred greater than 0 times to any of the verbal aggression questions, this was coded as “yes” for being the victim of verbal aggression (yes = 1; no = 0). If the participants answered “yes” to any of the physical assault questions, this was coded as “yes” for being the victim of physical assault (yes = 1; no = 0). See Table 3 for reported frequencies of victimization across the different sociodemographic characteristics.
Verbal aggression |
Physical assault |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Variable |
N |
% |
N |
% |
Gender |
||||
Men |
55 |
36.9 |
47 |
38.8 |
Women |
94 |
63.1 |
74 |
61.2 |
Total |
149 |
100.0 |
121 |
100.0 |
Born in Canada |
||||
Yes |
44 |
27.3 |
35 |
26.9 |
No |
117 |
72.7 |
95 |
73.1 |
Total |
161 |
100.0 |
130 |
100.0 |
Region |
||||
Calgary/Edmonton |
40 |
25.6 |
30 |
24.0 |
Ottawa |
17 |
10.9 |
14 |
11.2 |
Montréal |
44 |
28.2 |
35 |
28.0 |
Toronto |
32 |
20.5 |
26 |
20.8 |
Vancouver |
23 |
14.7 |
20 |
16.0 |
Total |
156 |
100.0 |
125 |
100.0 |
Age range |
||||
18–24 |
29 |
21.0 |
25 |
22.3 |
25–30 |
22 |
15.9 |
18 |
16.1 |
31–34 |
19 |
13.8 |
16 |
14.3 |
35–40 |
30 |
21.7 |
24 |
21.4 |
41–44 |
14 |
10.1 |
9 |
9.9 |
45–50 |
15 |
10.9 |
13 |
11.6 |
51+ |
9 |
6.5 |
7 |
6.3 |
Total |
138 |
100.0 |
112 |
100.0 |
Education |
||||
No certificate, diploma, or degree |
1 |
.7 |
0 |
1.0 |
High school diploma or equivalent |
13 |
8.7 |
9 |
7.1 |
Trades or apprenticeship certificate or diploma |
0 |
0.0 |
0 |
1.0 |
Certificate or diploma from a college, CEGEP, or other |
15 |
10.0 |
11 |
9.2 |
University certificate or diploma below bachelor’s degree |
7 |
4.7 |
4 |
4.6 |
Bachelor’s university degree |
53 |
35.3 |
49 |
35.7 |
Master’s university degree |
45 |
30.0 |
38 |
31.6 |
Degree in medicine, dentistry, veterinary medicine |
1 |
0.7 |
0 |
1.0 |
Doctorate degree |
15 |
10.0 |
12 |
8.7 |
Total |
150 |
100.0 |
123 |
100.0 |
Self-identified as visibly Muslim |
||||
Yes |
104 |
66.2 |
88 |
68.8 |
No |
53 |
33.8 |
40 |
31.3 |
Total |
157 |
100.0 |
128 |
100.0 |
Ethnic group |
||||
Asian (e.g., Afghan, Bangladeshi, Indian, Pakistani, etc.) |
41 |
25.8 |
37 |
28.7 |
European (e.g., Canadian, American, British, French, etc.) |
5 |
3.1 |
4 |
3.1 |
Maghreb (e.g., Algerian, Tunisian, Moroccan, etc.) |
34 |
21.4 |
28 |
21.7 |
Middle East (e.g., Arabic, Iranian, Iraqi, Lebanese, Syrian, etc.) |
30 |
18.9 |
22 |
17.1 |
Turk |
5 |
3.1 |
5 |
3.9 |
Sub-Saharan African (e.g., Senegalese, Ethiopian, Kenyan, etc.) |
11 |
6.9 |
9 |
7.0 |
Mixed Ethnicity |
33 |
20.8 |
24 |
18.6 |
Total |
159 |
100.0 |
129 |
100.0 |
Note: The percentage is the valid percent and is based on the total listed.
Psychological Distress
This measure consisted of 14 items. Participants were asked how they felt after being a victim of verbal aggression or physical assault. They were asked to answer Yes, No, or I don’t know to statements such as, “I felt afraid.” An exploratory factor analysis revealed that five items should be retained to create a factor of psychological distress (for example, the factor loadings were all greater than .5, which is considered fair–good; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). These items with frequencies in brackets were: 1) “I felt ashamed/guilty” (18%); 2) “I felt afraid” (39%); 3) “I felt depressed” (36%); 4) “I lost confidence” (30%); and 5) “I was less productive at work” (21%).
A factor score was generated for each participant based on these items (M = −0.03, SD = 1.00). The higher the score, the more distress the participant experienced.
Statistical Analyses
The first set of analyses included descriptive statistics for the demographic characteristics of those that reported being a victim of verbal aggression or physical assault. To address the research questions, three regression models were used. To address the first research questions (1a and 1b), two separate logistic regression models were estimated with verbal aggression and physical assault as the outcome variables. Gender; age group; education; ethnic group; self-identifying as visibly Muslim, Immigrant, or Canadian born; and the geographical region they are in were the independent variables. To address the second research question, a third linear regression model predicting psychological distress included the same independent variables with the addition of verbal aggression and physical assault as independent variables.
Results
Sample Demographics
The sample consisted of 231 participants2. The percentages reported in text are the rounded valid percentages (excluding missing cases). The sample had slightly more women (58%) than men (41%). Two participants reported not identifying with either man or woman (1%), but given the low case count, they were excluded from the regressions. Within the sample, 74% were not born in Canada. The ethnicities in order of decreasing frequency were Asian (27%), Maghreb (19%), Mixed Ethnicity (19%), Middle Eastern (16%), Sub-Saharan African (6%), Turk (8%), and Canadian/European (4%). Most of the participants were younger than 40 years old (72%). The sample had overall high levels of education. For example, 35% reported having a Bachelor’s degree, 32% reported having a Master’s degree, and 9% reported having a Doctoral degree. Approximately 1% of the sample reported not having any degree.
In terms of other characteristics, 64% of the sample reported that they self-identified as visibly Muslim. See Table 2 for more demographic information and how the key variables compare to the population of Muslims in Canada.
Demographics of Those That Experienced Hate Crimes
In the current sample, 74% reported being a victim of verbal aggression, and 63% reported being a victim of physical assault. To further understand the socio-demographics of those that experienced assaults, cross-tabulations were used (see Table 3). The key observations from the current sample are that higher levels of verbal aggression or physical assaults respectively were observed in women (63%; 61%), those not born in Canada (73%; 73%), persons residing in Montréal (28%; 28%), persons between the ages 18–24 (21%; 22%) and between the ages 35–40 (22%; 21%), those self-identifying as visibly Muslim (66%; 69%), and persons identifying as Asian (26%; 29%). See Table 3 for hate crime frequencies presented by these independent variables.
Predictors of Verbal Aggression (Model 1)
In the logistic regression model with verbal aggression as the outcome, the model fit was χ2 = 27.11, df = 15, p = .028, r2 = .15 (Cox & Snell R-Square). There were no significant associations between sociodemographic characteristics and having been a victim of verbal aggression. See Table 4a.
B |
S.E. |
OR |
|
---|---|---|---|
Gender (Man = 1; Woman = 0) |
−.584 |
.440 |
.557 |
Born in Canada (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
−.156 |
.542 |
.855 |
Age group |
−.063 |
.122 |
.939 |
Educational attainment |
−.014 |
.129 |
.986 |
Self-identified as visibly Muslim (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
.443 |
.439 |
1.558 |
Region Calgary and Edmonton |
.298 |
.670 |
1.347 |
Region Ottawa |
1.288 |
.902 |
3.627 |
Region Toronto |
1.412 |
.849 |
4.103 |
Region Vancouver |
1.670 |
.998 |
5.311 |
Ethnicity Asian |
−.628 |
1.323 |
.534 |
Ethnicity Maghreb |
.586 |
1.353 |
1.797 |
Ethnicity Middle East |
.255 |
1.331 |
1.290 |
Ethnicity Sub-Sahelian African |
.305 |
1.491 |
1.356 |
Ethnicity Turk |
−2.420 |
1.505 |
.089 |
Ethnicity Mixed |
.351 |
1.324 |
1.421 |
***p <0.001;
**p <0.01;
*p <0.05
Note: χ2 = 27.11, df = 15, p = .028, r2 = .15 (Cox & Snell R-Square). N = 166
The reference group for the region was Montreal. The reference group for the ethnicity was Canadian/European.
Predictors of Physical Assault (Model 2)
In the logistic regression model with physical assault as the outcome, the model fit was χ2 = 31.37, df = 15, p = .008, r2 = .17 (Cox & Snell R-Square). The only significant associations with the likelihood of being a victim of physical assault were self-identifying as visibly Muslim and living in Vancouver. Specifically, when the participant self-identified as visibly Muslim, they were three times more likely to have experienced a physical assault compared to those that did not self-identify as visibly Muslim (b = 1.18, OR = 3.27, p = .004). If the participant lived in Vancouver, they were nine times more likely to have experienced a physical assault compared to living in Montréal (b = 2.24, OR = 9.42, p = .009). See Table 4b.
B |
S.E. |
OR |
|
---|---|---|---|
Gender (Man = 1; Woman = 0) |
−.018 |
.393 |
.982 |
Born in Canada (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
−.387 |
.476 |
.679 |
Age group |
−.065 |
.111 |
.937 |
Educational attainment |
.170 |
.112 |
1.186 |
Self-identified as visibly (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
1.184** |
.408 |
3.268 |
Region Calgary and Edmonton |
.004 |
.555 |
1.004 |
Region Ottawa |
.794 |
.678 |
2.213 |
Region Toronto |
1.066 |
.666 |
2.904 |
Region Vancouver |
2.242** |
.864 |
9.417 |
Ethnicity Asian |
.713 |
1.040 |
2.039 |
Ethnicity Maghreb |
1.311 |
1.098 |
3.711 |
Ethnicity Middle East |
1.073 |
1.064 |
2.924 |
Ethnicity Sub-Sahelian African |
1.626 |
1.228 |
5.085 |
Ethnicity Turk |
−.992 |
1.243 |
.371 |
Ethnicity Mixed |
1.173 |
1.053 |
3.230 |
***p <0.001;
**p <0.01;
*p <0.05
Note: χ2 = 31.37, df = 15, p = .008, r2 = .17 (Cox & Snell R-Square). N = 164
The reference group for the region was Montreal. The reference group for the ethnicity was Canadian/European.
Impacts of Victimization
Participants were asked to report on how they felt psychologically after experiencing verbal aggression or physical assaults. The final measure as described above represents a factor score derived from the following feelings: feeling ashamed/guilty, feeling afraid; feeling depressed; feeling a loss of confidence; and feeling less productive at work.
Predictors of Psychological Distress (Model 3)
In the regression model with psychological distress as the outcome, the model fit was F(17, 104) = 3.099, df = 17, p <.001, r2 = .34. The significant associations with psychological distress were being a woman (coded as zero) (b = −.49, p = .007), self-identifying as visibly Muslim (b = .46, p = .018), residing in Vancouver (b = .84, p = .015), and being a victim of physical assault (b = .70, p < .001). See Table 4c.
B |
S.E. |
|
---|---|---|
Gender (Man = 1; Woman = 0) |
−.491** |
.178 |
Born in Canada (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
.236 |
.211 |
Age group |
.051 |
.052 |
Educational attainment |
−.068 |
.050 |
Self-identified as visibly Muslim (Yes = 1; No = 0) |
.461* |
.192 |
Region Calgary and Edmonton |
.310 |
.260 |
Region Ottawa |
.331 |
.295 |
Region Toronto |
.335 |
.299 |
Region Vancouver |
.843* |
.341 |
Ethnicity Asia |
−.621 |
.492 |
Ethnicity Maghreb |
−.800 |
.509 |
Ethnicity Middle East |
−.769 |
.486 |
Ethnicity Sub-Sahelian African |
−.540 |
.555 |
Ethnicity Turk |
.106 |
.609 |
Ethnicity Mixed |
−.483 |
.486 |
Verbal aggression |
.223 |
.377 |
Physical assault |
.700*** |
.199 |
***p <0.001;
**p <0.01;
*p <0.05
Note: F(17, 104) = 3.099, df = 17, p <.001, r2 = .34.
The reference group for the region was Montreal. The reference group for the ethnicity was Canadian/European.
Discussion
Hate crimes are harmful for victims as they tend to experience marked changes in their health and well-being after experiencing such events (Barry et al., 2020; Boeckmann, & Turpin-Petrosino, 2002; Gerstenfeld, 2011; Lefevor et al., 2018; Padela & Heisler, 2011). In particular, religiously motivated hate crimes and Islamophobia in Canada contribute to the distress experienced by Muslims living in Canada (Wilkins-Laflamme, 2018). This study sought to explore the distressing experiences of Canadian Muslims in response to hate crime victimization, providing a snapshot of the consequences of hate crimes against Muslims from different metropolitan areas across Canada.
The primary goal of the present study was to document and describe Canadian Muslims’ experiences of hate crime, including examining predictors of psychological distress after experiencing these hate crimes. The findings from the current study are in line with previous research about racially and religiously motivated hate crimes and the psychological distress associated with being victims of these crimes (Barry et al., 2020; Boeckmann, & Turpin-Petrosino, 2002; Gerstenfeld, 2011; Lefevor e al., 2018; Padela & Heisler, 2011). Specifically, in the current study, self-identifying as visibly Muslim resulted in a three-fold increase in probability of experiencing physical assault. Additionally, participants residing in Vancouver compared to Montréal also had a greater probability of experiencing physical assault. Though the increased victimization risk for those identifying as visibly Muslim is in line with previous research, the increased risk of experiencing physical assault for those living in Vancouver is surprising. For instance, recent legislative changes in Québec would have led to the expectation that Islamophobia may be more likely in the city of Montreal compared to Vancouver. This is particularly the case given the passing of Bill 21 in 2019, which prohibits wearing religious symbols in the workplace for civil servants, employees in positions of authority, and public sector teachers (https://www.thecanadianencyclopedia.ca/en/article/bill-21). Future research investigating the particular climate leading to more physical assaults of Muslims living in Vancouver is warranted.
Further, in the current study, psychological distress was predicted by being a woman, self-identifying as visibly Muslim, residing in Vancouver compared to Montreal, and being a victim of physical assault. Moreover, compared to Ameli and Merali (2014), the current sample exhibited higher incidence of physical assault (56.3% versus 11%). One such reason why this sample might have a higher incidence of physical assault could be due to the slight overrepresentation of Muslim women. This would align with statistics across Canada where Muslim women are most often the target of physical hate crimes (Moreau, 2021). Further, there may be a self-selection bias, where victims were more likely to respond to the survey because the hate crimes they experienced caused distress. This could have led to an overrepresentation of individuals in the sample who experienced hate crimes, and specifically physical assault. The present findings are also in line with previous research demonstrating that women are more likely to experience psychological distress (Viertiö et al., 2021). The results of the current study provide further evidence of psychological distress in response to hate crime victimization among Canadian women who are Muslim. Further, the present results suggest that the Muslim community, and Muslim women in particular, may need additional support to address issues related to the experience of hate crimes and the negative consequences, such as psychological distress. Lastly, the present findings help elucidate the intersectionality of religious, racioethnic, and gender identity in the context of experiencing hate crimes, and the impacts of such experiences on mental health.
Although this study provides an exploratory overview of the predictors and impacts of hate crimes against Muslims, there are several limitations. First, the sample is small. Despite more than 500 invitations having been sent and with a response rate of 45%, there were still only 230 participants. Second, the sample is not representative of the broader Muslim population, and therefore the results might not be generalizable. The participants had much higher educational attainment than the Canadian Muslim population (Statistics Canada, 2011). Those with lower educational attainment may have been more difficult to reach using a respondent-driven sampling methodology. Additionally, language proficiency might have represented an additional barrier for those not fluent in English or French. The sample, which drew more heavily from residents of Montreal, was also not representative of the distribution of the Muslim population across Canada, who tend to primarily reside in Toronto. This is partially due to difficulties with recruiting in that location. Though the results of this study might not reflect the experiences of the broader Muslim community, this study was able to obtain information from a hard-to-reach population, allowing us to better understand the impacts of hate crimes on Muslims.
Despite these limitations, this study demonstrated an important first step in establishing research examining the impacts of hate crime on Muslims and the psychological distress they experience, especially for Muslim women. The results presented here demonstrate the interconnectedness of factors such as gender and ethnicity in explaining hate crime experiences of Canadian Muslims. Future research and policy should consider these sociodemographic contexts to elucidate and address the impacts of hate crimes (such as psychological distress). By understanding the effects of hate crimes on Muslim populations, we can better address the needs of these communities and appropriately allocate resources in order to improve the health and well-being of these populations.
References
Allen, M. (2015). Police-reported Hate Crime in Canada. Ottawa: Canadian Centre for Justice Statistics. Catalogue no. 85–002-X.
Altemeyer, Bob. (1996). The Authoritarian Specter. Harvard University Press.
Ameli, S., & A. Merali (2014). Only Canadian: The Experience of Hate Moderated Differential Citizenship for Muslims. Wembley UK: Islamic Human Rights Commission.
Assari, S., & Lankarani, M. M. (2017). Discrimination and psychological distress: Gender differences among Arab Americans. Frontiers. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00023https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00023
Austen, I., & Smith, C. S. (2017, Jan. 29). Québec Mosque Shooting Kills at Least 6, and 2 Suspects Are Arrested. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/01/29/world/americas/quebec-city-mosque-shooting-canada.htmlhttps://www.nytimes.com/2017/01/29/world/americas/quebec-city-mosque-shooting-canada.html
Barry, V., Stout, M. E., Lynch, M. E., Mattis, S., Tran, D. Q., Antun, A., Ribeiro, M. J., Stein, S. F., & Kempton, C. L. (2020). The effect of psychological distress on health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Journal of Health Psychology, 25(2), 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105319842931https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105319842931
Boeckmann, R.J., & Turpin-Petrosino, C. (2002). Understanding the harm of hate crime. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 207–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4560.00257https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4560.00257
Canadian Islamic Congress (CIC). (2003). Islamic congress finds most police departments have incomplete data on rising tide of hate-motivated crimes. www.canadianislamiccongress.com/mc/media_communique.php?id=305.www.canadianislamiccongress.com/mc/media_communique.php?id=305
Canada Parliament. House of Commons (2017, Mar. 23). M-103 Private Members’ Business M-103 (Systemic racism and religious discrimination). Vote No. 237, 42nd Parliament, 1st session. Retrieved from: https://www.ourcommons.ca/Members/en/votes/42/1/237https://www.ourcommons.ca/Members/en/votes/42/1/237
CBC News (2020, Sept. 13). Police identify man stabbed to death outside Etobicoke mosque as Mohamed-Aslim Zafis, 58. CBC News. https://www.cbc.ca/news/canada/toronto/mosque-stabbing-1.5722305https://www.cbc.ca/news/canada/toronto/mosque-stabbing-1.5722305
Considine, C. (2017). The Racialization of Islam in the United States: Islamophobia, Hate Crimes, and “Flying while Brown”. Religions, 8(9), 165 https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090165https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090165
Council on American-Islamic Relations-Canada (CAIR-CAN) (2002). Canadian Muslims one year after 9/11. Ottawa ON: CAIR-CAN.
Drapeau, A., Marchand, A., & Beaulieu-Prévost, D. (2012). Epidemiology of psychological distress. In L. L’Abate (Eds.), Mental illnesses-understanding, prediction and control (pp. 105–134). https://doi.org/10.5772/30872https://doi.org/10.5772/30872
Environics Insitute. (2016). Survey Muslims in Canada 2016 - Environics Institute. https://www.environicsinstitute.org/docs/default-source/project-documents/survey-of-muslims-in-canada-2016/final-report.pdf?sfvrsn=fbb85533_2https://www.environicsinstitute.org/docs/default-source/project-documents/survey-of-muslims-in-canada-2016/final-report.pdf?sfvrsn=fbb85533_2
Gerstenfeld, P.B. (2011). Hate crimes: Causes, controls, and controversies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Heckathorn, D. (2002). Respondent-Driven Sampling II: Deriving valid population estimates from chain referral samples of hidden populations. Social Problems, 49(1), 11–34. https://doi.org/10.1525/sp.2002.49.1.11https://doi.org/10.1525/sp.2002.49.1.11
Heckathorn, D. (1997). Respondent-driven sampling: A new approach to the study of hidden populations. Social Problems, 44(2), p. 174–199. https://doi.org/10.2307/3096941https://doi.org/10.2307/3096941
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S.-L. T., … Zaslavsky, A. M. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32(6), 959–976. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291702006074https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291702006074
Lefevor, G.T., Park, S. Y. & Pedersen, T.R. (2018). Psychological distress among sexual and religious minorities: An examination of power and privilege. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Mental Health, 22(2), 90–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/19359705.2017.1418696https://doi.org/10.1080/19359705.2017.1418696
Lowe, S. R., Tineo, P., & Young, M. N. (2019). Perceived Discrimination and Major Depression and Generalized Anxiety Symptoms: In Muslim American College Students. Journal of Religion & Health, 58(4), 1136–1145. https://doi-org/10.1007/s10943-018-0684-1https://doi-org/10.1007/s10943-018-0684-1
Mercier-Dalphond, G., & Helly, D. (2021). Anti-Muslim Violence, Hate Crime, and Victimization in Canada: A Study of Five Canadian Cities. Canadian Ethnic Studies 53(1), 1–22. doi:10.1353/ces.2021.0000.10.1353/ces.2021.0000
Moreau, G. (2021). Police-reported hate crime in Canada, 2019. Juristat. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 85–002-X. https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/pub/85-002-x/2021001/article/00002-eng.pdf?st=zJ3sUCZj (accessed January 27, 2022).https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/pub/85-002-x/2021001/article/00002-eng.pdf?st=zJ3sUCZj
Padela, A.I., & Heisler, M. (2011). The association of perceived abuse and discrimination after September 11, 2001, with psychological distress, level of happiness, and health status among Arab Americans. American Journal of Public Health, 100(2), 284–291. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009
Perry, B. (2014). Gendered Islamophobia: Hate Crime against Muslim Women. Social Identities, 20(1), 74–89. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504630.2013.864467https://doi.org/10.1080/13504630.2013.864467
Perry, B., & Alvi, S. (2011). We are all vulnerable: The in terrorem effects of hate crime. International Review of Victimology, 18(1), 57–72. 10.1177/026975801142247510.1177/0269758011422475
Rousseau, C., Hassan, G., Moreau, N., & Thombs, B. D. (2011). Perceived discrimination and its association with psychological distress among newly arrived immigrants before and after September 11, 2001. American Journal of Public Health, 101(5), 909–915. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.173062https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.173062
Samari G. (2016). Islamophobia and Public Health in the United States. American journal of public health, 106(11), 1920–1925. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2016.303374https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2016.303374
Statistics Canada, Canadian Centre for Justice and Community Safety Statistics (2020). Uniform Crime Reporting Survey (3302). Retrieved from https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/daily-quotidien/210727/t001a-eng.htmhttps://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/daily-quotidien/210727/t001a-eng.htm
Statistics Canada (2011). National Household Survey, 2011 (99–010-X2011037). Retrieved from https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/catalogue/99-010-X2011037https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/en/catalogue/99-010-X2011037
Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2007) Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Pearson Education Inc.
Viertiö, S., Kiviruusu, O., Piirtola, M., Kaprio, J., Korhonen, T., Marttunen, M., & Suvisari, J. (2021). Factors contributing to psychological distress in the working population, with a special reference to gender difference. BMC Public Health 21, 611 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10560-yhttps://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10560-y
Westoll, N., & LeBel, J. (2021, June 7). 4 killed in London, Ont. attack likely targeted for being Muslim, police say. Global News. https://globalnews.ca/news/7928411/hyde-park-south-carriage-pedestrians-dead-london-ontario-police/https://globalnews.ca/news/7928411/hyde-park-south-carriage-pedestrians-dead-london-ontario-police/
Wilkins-Laflamme, S. (2018). Islamophobia in Canada: Measuring the Realities of Negative Attitudes Toward Muslims and Religious Discrimination. Canadian Review of Sociology, 55(1), 86–110. https://doi.org/10.1111/cars.12180https://doi.org/10.1111/cars.12180
Notes
- A visible minority is defined by Statistics Canada as “A visible minority refers to whether a person is a visible minority or not, as defined by the Employment Equity Act. The Employment Equity Act defines visible minorities as “persons, other than Aboriginal peoples, who are non-Caucasian in race or non-white in colour”. The visible minority population consists mainly of the following groups: South Asian, Chinese, Black, Filipino, Arab, Latin American, Southeast Asian, West Asian, Korean and Japanese.” (https://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p3Var.pl?Function=DEC'Id=45152) ⮭
- One participant didn’t answer enough of the questions to be included in any analyses and variables varied in missingness from 0.4%-32.5% of data; cases with missing data were excluded from the analyses. ⮭